4/9/2022

Iowa Gambling Task Scoring

  • PsyToolkit is a one-stop website. Get your questionnaire/experiment running quickly without hassle.

  • Design and run experiments or questionnaire surveys online (browser-based) or offline

  • Online data collection, storage, analysis, and download

  • No limits on numbers of questions or participants

  • Multi-lingual survey interface (including Spanish, French, German, 中文, and many more)

  • Extensive online documentation and YouTube channel with tutorial videos

  • It is suitable for teaching psychology, starting from secondary education

  • Ideal for student and professional research projects

  • Ideal for support in teaching cognitive or personality psychology

  • Rapid development possible by using questionnaires and experiments from the large library

  • Non-commercial and free to use

  • Made for and by academics

  • Reliably hosted by a professional data center

  • It uses free-software resources (click for acknowledgements)

  • There is a Frequently Asked Questions (FAQ) section

  • Excellent timing comparable to E-prime (demonstrated by independent research group)

The task was originally presented simply as the Gambling Task, or the 'OGT'. Later, it has been referred to as the Iowa gambling task and, less frequently, as Bechara's Gambling Task. The Iowa gambling task is widely used in research of cognition and emotion. A recent review listed more than 400 papers that made use of this paradigm. Cambridge Gambling Task (CGT) The Cambridge Gambling Task has been developed to assess decision making and risk taking behaviour outside a learning context. Administration time. Up to 18 minutes. The participant is presented with a row of ten boxes across the top of the screen: some are red and some are blue.

Services on Demand

  • The original Iowa Gambling Task studies decision making using a cards. The participant needs to choose one out of four card decks (named A,B,C, and D). The participant can win or loose money with each card.
  • Designed in 1994, the Iowa gambling task (IGT) has become one of the most complicated tasks used to study executive functions and emotionally driven decision making under uncertainty (Bechara et al., 1994, 1997, 1998, 1999, 2000).
  • Cited by SciELO

Related links

  • Similars in SciELO

Print version ISSN 1516-4446On-line version ISSN 1809-452X

http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-44462008005000009

BRIEF REPORT

Brazilian Portuguese version of the Iowa Gambling Task: transcultural adaptation and discriminant validity

Versão em português do Iowa Gambling Test: adaptação transcultural e validade discriminate

Leandro Fernandes Malloy-DinizI; Wellington Borges LeiteII; Paulo Henrique Paiva de MoraesI; Humberto CorreaIII; Antoine BecharaIV,V; Daniel FuentesVI

INeuropsychology and Neurosciences Research Group, Universidade FUMEC, Belo Horizonte (MG), Brazil
IIResearch Group of Attention Deficits (Gedahi), Learning Disorders and Hyperactivity, Belo Horizonte, (MG), Brazil
IIIUniversidade Federal de Minas Gerais (UFMG), Belo Horizonte (MG), Brazil
IVBrain and Creativity Institute, University of Southern California, Los Angeles (CA), USA
VDepartment of Neurology, College of Medicine, University of Iowa, Iowa City (Iowa), USA
VIPsychology & Neuropsychology Unit, Institute of Psychiatry, Clinical Hospital, Medical School, Universidade de São Paulo (USP), São Paulo (SP), Brazil

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: The Iowa Gambling Task is a neuropsychological task developed in English, most widely used to assess decision-making. The aim of this work was to adapt the Iowa Gambling Task to Brazilian Portuguese, compare it with the original version and assess its validity.
METHOD: We assessed 75 Brazilian adults divided into three groups: 1) 25 healthy volunteers holding the Proficiency Certificate in English tested using the English version of the Iowa Gambling Task; 2) 25 healthy volunteers who did not speak or read English tested using the Iowa Gambling Task-Portuguese; 3) 25 Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder subjects tested with the Iowa Gambling Task-Portuguese.
RESULTS: No difference between groups 1 and 2 was observed. Nonetheless, we found significant differences between Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder subjects and the other 2 groups on blocks 3, 4, 5, and on net score.
CONCLUSION: Our results are similar to those previously described in the literature concerning adults without neuropsychiatric diseases. Since those two versions were equivalent and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder subjects performed significantly worse than healthy volunteers we can conclude that the adaptation of the Iowa Gambling Task to Brazilian Portuguese is valid and can be used for research purposes in the Brazilian context.

Descriptors: Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity; Iowa; Task performance and analysis; Impulsive behavior; Cognition

RESUMO

OBJETIVO: Iowa Gambling Task é uma tarefa neuropsicológica originalmente desenvolvida em inglês, mais usada no mundo para avaliar o processo de tomada de decisões. Este estudo pretendeu adaptar o Iowa Gambling Task para o português, comparar a versão adaptada com a versão original em inglês e avaliar sua validade discriminante.
MÉTODO: Foram investigados 75 adultos brasileiros divididos em três grupos: 1) 25 voluntários sadios proficientes em inglês, avaliados com a versão original em inglês; 2) 25 voluntários sadios não-proficientes em inglês avaliados com o Iowa Gambling Task-português; 3) 25 adultos com Transtorno do Déficit de Atenção e Hiperatividade (avaliados com o Iowa Gambling Task-português.
RESULTADOS: Não houve diferenças entre os grupos 1 e 2. No entanto, encontramos diferenças entre os adultos com Transtorno do Déficit de Atenção e Hiperatividade e os outros dois grupos nos blocos 3, 4, 5 e no netscore.
CONCLUSÃO: Nossos resultados são semelhantes aos descritos na literatura. Considerando que as duas versões se mostraram equivalentes e os sujeitos com Transtorno do Déficit de Atenção e Hiperatividade desempenharam significativamente pior do que os controles, podemos concluir que a adaptação do Iowa Gambling Task para o português praticado no Brasil é válida e pode ser aplicada no contexto brasileiro.

Descritores: Transtorno da falta de atenção com hiperatividade; Iowa; Análise e desempenho de tarefas; Comportamento impulsivo; Cognição

Introduction

Impulsiveness is a core symptom of many psychiatric diseases and may be characterized as an umbrella construct encompassing characteristics such as swift action without conscious judgment, response without adequate thought, acting with less forethought, risk-taking without planning, and making up one's mind quickly.1 Barratt2 has devised an important model describing the multi-factorial nature of impulsiveness, grouping impulsiveness into motor impulsiveness (acting on the spur of the moment), attentional impulsiveness (not focusing on the task at hand), and non-planning impulsiveness (orientation towards the present, rather than the future).

Barratt's classification of impulsiveness presents some convergence with Bechara's model,3 which argues that there is a functional and structural difference between motor impulsivity (related to inhibition of pre-potent responses), and decision-making (also referred to as “cognitive impulsivity.”) that seems analogous with Barratt's non-planning impulsivity (behavior orientation to the present rather than to the future). Bechara also discusses another type of impulsivity, concerning the ability to inhibit irrelevant information held in working memory and to focus on the task at hand. This type of impulsivity may also be analogous with the attentional impulsivity aspect described by Barratt (lack of concentration).

Iowa Gambling Task Scoring Guide

The neuropsychological assessment of the motor component of impulsiveness has been carried out by use of response-inhibition tasks such as go-no-go tasks, the stop signal reaction time test, and perseverative errors on tasks such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.4 Attentional impulsiveness can also be assessed using, for instance, omission errors on tasks such as the Continuous Performance Task.5 Concerning non-planning impulsiveness, up until the 1990s there was a lack of tools to assess non-planning impulsiveness. At this time, Bechara et al.6 developed the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), a paradigm that is believed to model real-life decision-making and assess cognitive (non-planning) impulsiveness.

The IGT has been used both in neurological and neuropsychiatric contexts. Decision-making problems has been identified on different populations, such as patients with ventromedial orbitofrontal lesions,4,6 Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder patients,5,7-9 violent suicide attempters,10 and alcoholics subjects.11

Iowa gambling task scoring

Briefly, on IGT, subjects have to choose one card at a time from four available decks (A, B, C and D). The task requires subjects to make 100 choices (100 trials), and in each trial, subjects may win or lose a certain amount of money. Two of the decks (A and B) yield high immediate gain, but in the long run they incur higher loss, and thus are disadvantageous decks. In contrast, the other two decks (C and D) yield relatively lower gains, but in the long run incur smaller losses, and thus are advantageous. Over the course of the trials, healthy volunteers learn to avoid the disadvantageous decks in preference for the advantageous ones. To measure performance, choices are divided into five blocks, with twenty choices each. For each block, a net score (number of cards selected from the advantageous (good) decks minus the disadvantageous (bad) decks) is obtained. A total net score from all blocks is also obtained.

The IGT was developed in English. In spite of its ease of comprehension, IGT use by individuals of other native languages could be hampered if they have cognitive/intellectual deficits, and a low educational and social profile. This study had two objectives. The first aim was to adapt the IGT to the Brazilian Portuguese (IGT-Br) and compare it with the original English version. The second aim of this study was to evaluate the construct validity of the Brazilian Portuguese adaptation of the IGT, using it to assess individuals diagnosed with ADHD and then comparing results with healthy volunteers. Our hypothesis is that there would be no difference in performance on the original IGT and the Brazilian version of the IGT. We also hypothesized that the Brazilian version of the IGT would be valid to measure the decision-making deficits present in a clinical population.

Method

1. Translation of the IGT

A researcher P.H.M (proficient in English) made the translation of IGT's text. A professional translator made the back translation. Finally, the Brazilian Portuguese translation and the back-translation were submitted to a judgment of two other researchers LFMD and DF (both proficient in English).

2. Participants

We assessed seventy-five Brazilian adults divided into three groups:

1) Twenty-five healthy volunteers (17 women and eight men) holding the Proficiency Certificate in English were recruited through advertisements in English Schools. We accepted Cambridge Certificate in Advanced English, Cambridge Certificate of Proficiency in English, International English Language Testing System, and MICHIGAN Examination for the Certificate of Proficiency in English. This group was assessed through the original version of IGT.

2) Twenty-five healthy volunteers (15 women and 10 men) who did not speak or read English, were recruited through local advertisements at universities. This group was tested using the IGT-Br.

3) Twenty-five ADHD subjects (10 women and 15 men) consecutively admitted to the GEDAHI (Learning, Attention, and Hyperactivity Disorders Research Group) were invited to participate in this study answering the IGT-Br. All subjects were diagnosed on adulthood and were of combined type.

All subjects of the three groups had at least 8 years of formal education: 13.3% had a diploma of elementary school (corresponding to eight years of schooling), 20% had a diploma of high school (corresponding to eleven years of schooling), 26.7% had incomplete university degree (corresponding to at least eleven years of schooling) and 40% had completed university degree.

Candidates from groups 1 and 2 were interviewed through the Mini-Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI 5.0)12 to exclude psychiatric disorders. The MINI 5.0 encompasses 17 disorders described on axis 1 of DSM-IV (including Affective Disorders, Psychotic disorders, ADHD), suicide risk and antisocial personality disorder.

ADHD subjects were diagnosed according to DSM-IV criteria and the diagnosis was carried out by at least two independent clinicians (neurologist or psychiatrist and neuropsychologist). On the day of the neuropsychological assessment, none of the ADHD individuals had used methylphenidate or any other medication used in the treatment of symptoms of inattention and/or hyperactivity. None of the patients were using long-lasting methylphenidate.

The Demographic data, including intelligence through Raven's progressive Matrices13 are reported in Table 1.

The power analyzes of the sample size was based on a difference detected between means score of IGT's net score detected between groups 2 and 3 (d = 16). We used the following formula to compute the power of the sample

where n = sample size (75) σ = standard deviation (24) and d = difference between means for a significance level of 5% (z1-α = 1.96), the power of the sample was of 96.6 (z1-β = 2.12).

The FUMEC University Ethics Review Committee had approved the study protocol (REF 262/2007). All participants signed informed consents before participating in this study.

Development of the IGT-Br:

Iowa Gambling Task Scoring Rules

The IGT-Br was developed in the Windows environment by the author P.H.P.M. The translation was made by two researchers who were proficient in English, and the resulting translation was subsequently approved by two other researchers also proficient in English. The adaptation maintained the same visual characteristics and schedule of reinforcement/punishment used in the original version.4

3. Statistical analyses

To compare the judgment of two raters about the sentence-by-sentence translation of the instructions (e.g. “I want you to select one card at a time from any deck you choose”) and feedbacks (e.g. “You won $ 100”) of IGT we use the Cohen Kappa's coefficient

Where Pr(a) is the relative observed agreement among raters, and Pr(e) is the probability that agreement among ratters is due to chance.

Our analyses consisted of one-way ANOVA to compare group performances on the IGT (Blocks 1 to 5 and net score) and employed the Bonferroni test to specify group differences. We use α = 0.05 as the significance level.

Results

Considering the translation of the IGT's text to Brazilian Portuguese, we found Cohen Kappa's coefficient ranging from 0.8 to 1 indicating that both raters consider the translation to the Portuguese was appropriate.

The mean of years of formal education of the sample was about 13 years. This is equivalent to college level in Brazilian Educational Classification. Nonetheless, since our sample was composed by subjects with at least 8 years of formal education, we consider this version useful for subjects with at least this same level of education (equivalent to “8th grade” or elementary school in Brazil).

No statistical differences between volunteers and patients were found regarding demographic profiles (Table 1).

Iowa Gambling Task Scoring

No difference between groups 1 and 2 was observed, in terms of number of choices of the different card decks and on the pattern of choices over five successive blocks of 20 trials (Table 1; Figure 1). Nonetheless, we found that ADHD subjects performed worse than groups 1 and 2 on Block 3, Block 4, Block 5 and on net score (Table 1).

Discussion

Our results are similar to those previously described in the literature concerning adults without neuropsychiatric disorders.4,6 Healthy volunteer subjects attained a learning curve over the course of the trials, making more advantageous choices throughout the task. No difference was found between group 1 and 2, or in equivalence between the IGT and IGT-Br.

ADHD subjects performed significantly worse than those from Groups 1 and 2, suggesting that individuals with ADHD make less advantageous decisions than those without ADHD. These data points to discriminant validity of the IGT-Br. Our results were similar to those of an earlier investigation using the original English version of the test to assess ADHD children,9 adolescents7-8 and adults.5

Using a child's adaptation of IGT, Garon et al.,9 have found deficits in decision-making in children with pure ADHD (n = 11), as compared to children with ADHD associated with internalizing symptoms (n = 11), and as compared to matching controls (n = 21). Pure ADHD subjects made more disadvantageous choices on the last two blocks of choices (total of 4 blocks). Using the IGT, Toplak et al.,7 have showed a pattern of poor decision-making in adolescents with ADHD (n = 36), in comparison to controls (n = 34). These results were similar to those reported by Ernst et al.,8 in a study comparing adolescents with disruptive behaviors (including ADHD) to matching controls. Malloy-Diniz et al.,5 have also found differences between adults with ADHD (n = 50) and healthy controls (n = 51) on IGT. In that study, individuals with ADHD made more disadvantageous choices than healthy controls and the pattern of choices were similar to the present study with statistically significant differences on the third, fourth and fifth blocks.

The pattern of differences on the last IGT's blocks between healthy volunteers and ADHD subjects has been consistently shown regardless the age of subject.5,9 This finding can suggest that ADHD subject's fails to learn from experience and also could represent a pattern of non-planning impulsivity. In agreement with this hypothesis, Malloy-Diniz et al.,5 have found a negative correlation between the scores on non-planning impulsivity measured by the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) and the net score of choices on the last blocks of IGT.

Our study has some limitations. We did not use random assignment to the English or Portuguese version of IGT in the bilingual group. We also did not use case matching criteria to select subjects for the ADHD group. Furthermore, we did not make comparisons between inattentive and combined subtypes of ADHD, or between ADHD subgroups with different comorbid disorders. The effects of different ADHD subtypes and comorbid conditions on the IGT's performance should be addressed in future studies to clarify the generalization of the use of IGT to identify decision-making deficits on subjects according to its subtype and comorbidity. These limitations hinder the generalization of our results.

Iowa Gambling Task Net Score

These results are compatible with the hypothesis of Decision-Making Deficits in ADHD subjects. Therefore, we can conclude that the adaptation of the IGT to Brazilian Portuguese is valid and can be used for research purposes in the Brazilian context. Future studies with the IGT-Br on another clinical populations and subjects with low educational level could add more information about the validity of these tasks on our context.

Iowa Gambling Task Scoring

Iowa Gambling Task Scoring

Acknowledgement

The Brazilian adaptation does not substitute the original version and information on its use and license can be obtained from Antoine Bechara, University of Iowa. The Gambling Task is copyrighted and cannot be published or distributed without prior permission.

References

Iowa

1. Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(11):1783-93. [ Links ]

2. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. J Clin Psychol. 1995;51(6):768-74. [ Links ]

3. Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Emotion, decision and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 2000;10(3):295-307. [ Links ]

4. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H. Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain. 2000;123(Pt 11):2189-202. [ Links ]

5. Malloy-Diniz L, Fuentes D, Leite WB, Correa H, Bechara A. Impulsive behavior in adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: characterization of attentional, motor and cognitive impulsiveness. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2007;13(4):693-8. [ Links ]

Iowa Gambling Task Scoring

6. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition. 1994;50(1-3):7-15. [ Links ]

7. Ernst M, Grant SJ, London ED, Contoreggi CS, Kimes AS, Spurgeon L. Decision making in adolescents with behavior disorders and adults with substance abuse. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(1):33-40. [ Links ]

8. Toplak ME, Jain U, Tannock R. Executive and motivational processes in adolescents with Attention-Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Behav Brain Funct. 2005;1(1):8. [ Links ]

9. Garon N, Moore C, Waschbusch DA. Decision making in children with ADHD only, ADHD-anxious, depressed, and control children using a child version of the Iowa Gambling Task. J Atten Dis. 2006;9(4):607-19. [ Links ]

10. Jollant F, Bellivier F, Leboyer M, Astruc B, Torres S, Verdier R, Castelnau D, Malafosse A, Courtet P. Impaired decision making in suicide attempters. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(2):304-10. [ Links ]

11. Verdejo-García A, Rivas-Pérez C, Vilar-López R, Pérez-García M. Strategic self-regulation, decision-making and emotion processing in poly-substance abusers in their first year of abstinence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;86(2-3):139-46. [ Links ]

Iowa gambling task scoring rules

12. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, Dunbar GC. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59(Suppl 20):22-33. [ Links ]

13. Raven J, Raven JC, Court JH. Manual for Raven's Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. Section 1: General Overview. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment; 2003. [ Links ]

Correspondence
Leandro F. Malloy-Diniz
Health Sciences School, Universidade FUMEC
Nova Lima Campus, Minas Gerais, MG, Brazil
E-mail: malloy.diniz@gmail.com

Iowa Gambling Task Scoring Rubric

Submitted: July 21, 2007
Accepted: January 21, 2008

Iowa Gambling Task Scoring Leaders

Financing: None
Conflict of interests: None